DOI: 10.7256/2454-0722.2017.4.24335
Дата направления статьи в редакцию:
03-10-2017
Дата публикации:
16-01-2018
Аннотация:
Предметом исследования данной статьи являются национальные автостереотипы россиян. Перестройка и последовавшая за ней социальная трансформация значимо повлияла на коллективные представления, затронув систему этноидентичности, к структуре которой относятся этнические автостереотипы. Необходимость изучить направление произошедшей трансформации российских этностереотипов и определила предмет исследования статьи. Авторы проводят не только теоретическое, но и эмпирическое исследование содержательного наполнения автостереотипов россиян. В эмпирическом исследовании были использованы два метода сбора данных: проективный рисунок и анкетирование. Такой выбор методов позволил получить данные сразу двух пластов: установок сознания (анкетирование) и бессознательного (проективный рисунок). Научная новизна исследования определяется, в первую очередь, именно совмещением методов, позволившим изучить не только осознаваемые автостереотипные представления, но и неосознаваемые, трудно вербализуемые. Полученный результат подтвердил правильность такого подхода, показав: между осознанными и бессознательными автостереотипами россиян существуют значительные различия. Если по итогам анкетирования исследуемые описывают представителей своего народа в целом позитивно, то обработка проективных рисунков показывает, что бессознательные автостереотипы россиян имеют довольно большое количество негативных коннотаций.
Ключевые слова:
автостереотипы, автостереотипы россиян, исследование автостереотипов, бессознательные автостереотипы, осознаваемые автостереотипы, ментальность, этническая ментальность, национальное самовосприятие, этническое самовосприятие, самовосприятие русских
Abstract: The subject of the article is the national auto-stereotypes of Russians. Perestroika and the ensuing social transformation significantly influenced the collective representation, touching upon the system of ethnic identity to the structure which includes ethnic auto-stereotypes. The need to examine the direction of this transformation of the Russian ethnic stereotype was the reason which defined the subject of the article. The authors not only theoretical but also empirical study of the substantive content of the auto-stereotypes of Russians. In the empirical investigation two methods were used for data collection: projective drawing and questionnaire.This choice of methods allowed us to obtain data from two strata: units of consciousness (questionnaire) and unconscious (projective drawing). The scientific novelty of research is determined, first and foremost, it is a combination of the methods, allowing to study not only autostereotype conscious representation and unconscious, it is difficult verbalize. The result confirmed the correctness of this approach, showing: between conscious and unconscious auto-stereotypes of Russians there is considerable variation. If the result of questionnaire describes the representatives of the people as a whole positively, the processing of projective drawings shows that unconscious auto-stereotypes of Russians have quite a large number of negative connotations.
Keywords: auto-stereotypes, auto-stereotypes of Russians, study of auto-stereotypes, unconscious auto-stereotypes, auto-stereotypes perceived, mentality, ethnic mentality, national self-presentation, ethnic self-perception, self-perception of Russian
Introduction
People of different cultures tend to develop general characteristics of other social groups and about themselves. Those typical features are usually regarded as stereotypes in everyday discourse as well as in the social science tradition. Throughout the short history of studying stereotypes, different approaches to understanding the nature of this concept have been introduced. View on stereotypes as cultural and collective beliefs has been accepted since the very appearance of this phenomenon. Later on, however, individual approach has been developed by social psychologists who started to be preoccupied with processes of categorization (see, for example, Schneider D. J.[15]). As a consequence of the dominance of the social psychology in the field of study of stereotypes, the role of culture in determinacy of stereotypes was often disregarded. In the case of national stereotypes, however, culture and consensus cannot be underestimated and stereotypes should be viewed as a collection of cultural beliefs shared among the members of a society. At the same time, the national configuration of stereotypes can be seen as hindering or helping the progress of the nation in civilizational dynamics. [1, c. 385-386.] The content of stereotypical perceptions of Russians is the main focus of the present article.
Stereotypes are deeply embedded in the fabric of culture, and due to this fact historical sources provide rich material for the interpretation and the reconstruction of popular beliefs and opinions. However, the first scientific notion of this human tendency was introduced only in the XX century. Walter Lippmann, was the first to use the term ‘stereotype’, and since then, the name of this American journalist is cited almost in every work dedicated to this phenomenon. The popularity of this definition in everyday language can be explained by the multidimensional character of stereotypes which play an important role in the construction of the social world. Originally, Lippmann, in his famous book Public Opinion [12], described stereotypes as ‘pictures in our heads’. In the classic study of discrimination The Nature of Prejudice (1954), Gordon W. Allport gave the following definition of stereotypes: “…a stereotype is an exaggerated belief associated with category” [9, p.191]. Thus, in Allport’s view stereotypes are perceived as inaccurate mental generalizations or fixed ideas about the social world. In the mainstream of social science at that time such adjectives as wrong and inadequate were frequently used to describe stereotypes.
This definition of stereotypes was highly influential until the existence of correspondence between characteristics of groups and the beliefs which others held of them was discovered. The notion that stereotypes bear the “kernel of truth” and therefore resemble real characteristics changed the attitude toward this phenomenon in social science tradition (see, for example, Klineberg in McDonald [13]). Still, in everyday discourse stereotypes are usually viewed as inaccurate representations.
National stereotypes by definition refer to people’s judgements about other people and their cultures. Stereotypes about other nations comprise of number of beliefs which construct national character. Traditionally national character was viewed as “the average (or central tendency) of the characteristics of individuals” [14, p.71]. This means that the majority of the population of a given community possesses a set of typical traits, however, there can be modes in this common pattern. National character implies the modal characteristics – traits that occur with the highest frequency. However, these modal traits are largely constructed with the help of stereotypes.
Studies of national characteristics as typical traits of Russian national character have a long tradition of analysis in social sciences (see, for example, Gorer G., Rickman J.[11]; Peabody D.[1985]; Peabody D., Shmelev A.G., Andreyeva M.K., Gramenitskiy A.E. [5]; Kasyanova K.[2]; Melnikova A.A. [3],[4], to name just a few). Within the social sciences, studies on Russian national characteristics started to appear in mainstream anthropology after the Second World War. Anthropologists tried to find typical features and summarize them into national character. Although it was not always possible for the researchers to visit those countries and conduct a study (as it was the case with the Soviet Union), literature and historical analysis as well as examinations of traditions were helpful in investigating closed communities.
This type of study was carried out by British anthropologists Geoffrey Gorer and John Rickman in 1949. They proposed the so-called ‘swaddling hypothesis’ in order to understand the nature of the Russian character. It was suggested that the peasants’ practice of rapping up infants tightly (to keep arms and legs immobile) and unwrapping them only for feeding or playing results in the construction of specific features. Periods of anger and passions which transform into exhaustion, loneliness and depression were attributed to Russians based on the assumption that restrictions posed on a person in early childhood would have an effect on them as adults (Gorer G., Rickman J. [11]).
Clearly, the Russian character cannot be fully explained by childhood experiences; it is a product of the interaction of a variety of geographical, social economic and cultural events. The acceptance of Byzantine Christianity and the experiences of the Mongol invasion are only few factors that accounted for the establishment of specific Russian characteristics. A high level of emotionality, extremism, unpredictability and maximalism are usually attributed to Russians. However, all these traits can be juxtaposed with others, such as fatalism, passivity, submissiveness and laziness. Those contradictions of the national character were stressed by Russian philosophers (e.g., Lossky, Berdyaev, and Solovyev). Berdyaev, for example, acknowledged clashing principles that can be found in the foundation of the ‘Russian soul’: the indigenous pagan (Dionysian) and the ascetic (Christian Orthodox). According to Berdyaev [10], this antagonism is the source of the contradictory traits of the Russian people: goodness and brutality, individualism and collectivism, rebellion and slavery.
Different characteristics attributed to Russians can be found in folktales where the Russians described themselves as kind, honest and strong. Yet, in the same fairy tale one can find depictions of Russians as drunkards, liars and cheaters. Even the most popular character in Russian folktales is called Ivan the Fool – the name which in itself conotates certain personality traits.
A variety of characteristics ascribed to Russians can have different forms, and literature provides another source of information. Fedor Dostoyevskiy in his Diary of a Writer stated that a “Russian man is too broad, it wouldn’t be bad if he became narrower” (Dostoyevsky in Segal B. [16, p. 232]). This observation reveals the difficulties in examining Russian characteristics. However, the attempts have been made on different accounts. The inside view of the characteristics of Russians can be observed in the works of the most prominent Russian writers (for example, Tolstoy, Dostoyevsky, Chekhov, and Leskov). In the novels by Dostoyevsky and Tolstoy Russian peasants usually were attributed such traits as patience, modesty, goodness and moral purity. However, striking contradictions can be found between these descriptions and the observations made by other Russian writers. Chekhov, for instance, described Russian peasants as distrustful, cruel and idle. By no means, Russia can be regarded as a homogeneous society, therefore, the image of this nationality can very, as this is the case in the writers’ portrayals of Russians.
Writers and thinkers have greatly contributed to the construction of Russian stereotypes. The question arises then, how do Russians view themselves? Do the stereotypes promoted by historical events and literature traditions persist or do they crystallize into other characteristics? A survey conducted by Sikevich (1996) indicates that the respondents view a typical Russian as kind, tolerant, hospitable and hard-working. This does not correspond with characteristics attributed to Russians by anthropologists, philosophers and writers. It can be explained by the fact that that the image of one’s own group (auto-stereotype) is usually described in more positive terms.
Auto- and hetero-stereotypes are studied as means for investigation ethnic self-consciousness (Sikevich Z.V. [6]; Smolina T.L.[7]) and mutual perceptions of groups as a result of intercultural contact during cross-cultural adaptation (Ward C., Bochner S., Furnham A. [17]; Smolina T.L., Melnikova A.A. [8]).
Methodology of the Research
In the present study of auto-stereotypes two different types of methods – projective drawing and questionnaire – were adopted. The projective drawing technique as a research tool is frequently used in psychology when subjects are asked to draw different objects and thus express their attitudes and feelings. In the case of current research respondents were given the task of drawing typical Russian. This drawing technique allows a research to diminish problems with language. Language is both a vehicle for and an obstacle to the cross-cultural research due to the fact that specific methods designed for one country if applied to the other cultural setting without an appropriate translation could produce unreliable results. The use of drawings reduces this danger, as it is administered to the respondents in an unlimited format and subjects have the freedom of self-expression and reflexivity. Moreover the main advantage of this research technique is that it can tap into the respondents’ own images. However, the ambiguity of interpretation is one of the significant disadvantages of this method when presenting the results obtained from the drawings. In the current study the drawing technique will be analyzed through content analysis.
The other data was collected from the questionnaire. The short questionnaire was constructed specifically to address the issue of national stereotypes. One of the question was constructed in the form of semantic differential – the technique which is comprised of a set of polar traits-adjectives that are presented to the respondents on the basis of a seven-point scale. Subjects are asked to check the items that are closest to their position. In the present research twelve pairs of features were constructed. On the whole, the questionnaire was produced for the purpose of finding out additional information about respondents as well as to support the empirical evidence received from the drawings. The use of drawings and then juxtaposing them with information gathered through the questionnaire allows a second alternative reading due to the combination of received results.
Procedure
A sample of the Russian students from Herzen State Pedagogical University (St. Petersburg, Russia) took part in the research. This target group consisted of psychology students aged between 18 and 27. Fifty-two Russian respondents participated in the study: 33 women and 19 men.
The surveys were distributed simultaneously to the subjects for completion. First, respondents were asked to fill in the questionnaire. Upon its completion, the respondents were asked to think about and draw a picture of a typical Russian. The whole procedure took from 20 to 30 minutes.
Method of Analysis
Results obtained through the semantic differential technique are presented in average scores of the trait-adjective attributed to different nationalities. The data gathered from the drawings were analyzed using the method of content analysis. The element of the drawing was regarded as a coding unit. Categories were not invented or theoretically derived; they were drawn from the material (elements of pictures). The classification scheme contains ten different categories (i.e. Political and national symbols, Leisure time symbols, Labor symbols, Advertising symbols, Plants and animals, Food and drinks, etc.).
The elements which were not included in the categories were analyzed qualitatively. For instance, gender, type of clothes and the general appearance of the depicted person were taken into account. The drawing technique proved to be a valid and reliable research tool because it can be analyzed through content analysis. Second, it can be interpreted both quantitatively and qualitatively. The respondents can express not only the views they were conscious of (through the questionnaire) but also the attitudes they were unaware of (through the drawings).
The self-evaluations of Russians
The analysis of auto-stereotypes consist of two stages. First, the results obtained from the drawings will be analyzed qualitatively and quantitatively. Second, the findings received from the survey will be presented and compared from the previously mentioned data.
During the first step of our analysis, the auto-stereotypes of the Russian respondents will be assessed through the examination of drawings. It should be stated from the beginning that there were no abstract drawings; all of the subjects drew a person as a typical representative of the Russian nationality. However, although the majority of the respondents depicted a man, there were several drawings by female subjects who drew a woman. More specifically, five percent of the female respondents perceived a typical Russian as a female person. This tendency to differentiate the gender in the stereotypical perception was noticeable only in the drawings of the female respondents.
Another issue which is worth mentioning concerns the general appearance of the depicted person. While the facial expression was not taken into account due to the ambiguity of interpretation, the type of clothes and its elements were carefully observed. There was a considerable amount (24%) of drawings where a typical Russian was depicted in winter clothes (e.g. fur coat, fur hat, gloves). Less frequently respondents drew a person in a military form (5%), sportswear (2%), business suit (2%) and traditional national costume (1%). More often, however, a typical Russian in the drawings was presented around different plants and animals, as is shown in Table 1.
Table 1. Auto-stereotypes of Russian Respondents (%)
Category
|
Code
|
%
|
Political and national symbols
|
PN
|
18
|
Leisure time symbols
|
LT
|
12,5
|
Economic life of a society
|
E
|
1,5
|
Labor symbols
|
L
|
6
|
Technical objects
|
T
|
0
|
Advertising symbols
|
AD
|
4,5
|
Plants and animals
|
PA
|
26,5
|
Food and drinks
|
FD
|
4,5
|
Alcohol
|
A
|
22
|
Negative symbols
|
N
|
4,5
|
According to the findings, category ‘Plants and animals’ dominates in the perception of a typical Russian by Russian subjects. The drawings of bears, cats, dogs, horses as well as different types of trees were observed in 26,5% of cases. The second group of symbols which appears significant for the auto-stereotypes of Russians is the category ‘Alcohol’ (22%). Besides the drawings which depicted a person with a bottle of vodka or beer, 18% of the drawings contained political and national symbols. The latter include both Russian and Soviet symbols (Soviet and Russian flags, sickle and hammer, Soviet star), which shows that the Soviet heritage still dominates in the perception of a stereotyped Russian. Interestingly enough, the next popular category is the category of leisure time symbols (12,5%), where respondents draw various musical instruments (accordion, balalaika) and books (one of the books was titled “War and Peace”). Objects representing labor (spade, scythe, hammer, axe) were also present in 6% of drawings.
As it was noted earlier, one of the questions in the survey was presented in the form of a scale, where students were asked to rate how typical a certain characteristic is for a Russian on a range from one to seven, where a score “one” represented the more favorable direction. Alternatively, a tendency to respond in a more unfavorable direction resulted in scoring closer to number seven. Table 2 illustrates the auto-stereotypes of Russian respondents on the bipolar scale basis.
Table 2. Bipolar Scale Characteristics of a Typical Russian by Russian Respondents (Mean Values)
Characteristics
|
Russian
|
brave / cowardly
|
2,2
|
active / passive
|
3,1
|
hard-working / lazy
|
3,2
|
educated / uneducated
|
3,9
|
tolerant / intolerant
|
3,7
|
sociable / unsociable
|
2,2
|
frank / devious
|
3
|
modest / conceited
|
4,2
|
kind / mean
|
2,4
|
generous / stingy
|
2,5
|
patriotic / unpatriotic
|
2,5
|
well-mannered / primitive
|
3,6
|
The Russian is believed to excel in courage, sociability, kindness and patriotism. Overall, it cannot be said that Russians view themselves negatively; there are no ratings near to the score “seven”. Moreover, the closest number to the unfavorable direction (4,2 – modest / conceited). Nevertheless, a score of 4 is usually considered the middle point or the constant in the seven-point scale. It can be concluded that auto-stereotypes of Russians hold a favorable (positive) direction according to the results of the survey.
A possible explanation of the discrepancies between the data – drawings and questionnaire – can be the following. Such characteristics as drunkenness and poverty were not presented in the questionnaire and the possible scoring of those features is not known. The images of guns that appeared in the drawings of Russian respondents can be interpreted as an indication of aggressiveness – another negative characteristic. Thus, the drawing analysis provides the images which symbolize certain characteristics and reveal other dimensions of the auto-stereotypes for further interpretation.
Conclusions
The empirical investigation provided a range of findings in regard to auto- stereotypes of Russians. First, it cannot be said that the auto-stereotypes of the Russians are strictly positive. On the contrary, the auto-stereotypes contain a rather large amount of negative connotations according to the analysis of projective drawings. The qualitative interpretation of the drawings demonstrated the existence of gender differences in stereotyping. In particular, Russian male respondents never drew a woman as a representative of the other nationality. Female subjects, on the other hand, in some cases depicted a woman when given the same assignment. Overall, it can be concluded that this study not only offered an enquiry into the content of stereotypes, but also posed questions which appear to be challenging for future investigation into the nature of stereotypes.
Библиография
1. Горшкова В.В., Мельникова А.А. Своеобразие национальной ментальности России как основание ее цивилизационной динамики// Современные глобальные вызовы и национальные интересы. XVI Международные Лихачевские научные чтения. Российская академия наук, Конгресс петербургской интеллигенции, Санкт-Петербургский гуманитарный университет профсоюзов, при поддержке Министерства иностранных дел РФ. СПб: из-во СПбГУП, 2016. С. 385-386. 656 с.
2. Касьянова К. О русском национальном характере. М.: Академический Проект; Екатеринбург: Деловая книга, 2003. – 560 с.
3. Мельникова А.А. Язык как текст национальной культуры и форма репрезентации ее смысловых оснований. Диссертация на соискание ученой степени доктора культурологии / Санкт-Петербургский Гуманитарный университет профсоюзов. Санкт-Петербург, 2014. 381 с.
4. Мельникова А.А. Язык и национальный характер. Взаимосвязь структуры языка и ментальности. СПб.: Речь, 2003. – 320 с.
5. Пибоди Д., Шмелев А.Г., Андреева М.К., Граменицкий А.Е. Психосемантический анализ стереотипов русского характера: кросскультурный аспект // Вопросы психологии, №3. Москва, 1993.-С. 101 – 109.
6. Сикевич З.В. Национальное самосознание русских. М.: Институт «Открытое общество», 1996. – 204 с.
7. Смолина Т.Л. Этническое самосознание как социально-психологический феномен // Известия РГПУ им. А. И. Герцена, №171. СПб.: Издательство РГПУ им. А. И. Герцена, 2014.-С. 301-306.
8. Смолина Т.Л., Мельникова А.А. Психология кросс-культурной адаптации. СПб.: Издательско-Торговый дом «Скифия», 2017. – 304 с.
9. Allport, Gordon W., 1979 , The Nature of Prejudice. Reading, Mass.: Addison Wesley.
10. Berdyaev, Nikolay, 1971, Russkaya Ideya (The Russian Idea). Paris: YMCA Press.
11. Gorer G., Rickman J., 1949, The People of Great Russia: a Psychological Study. London.
12. Lippmann, Walter, 1946, Public Opinion. New York: Harcourt Brace.
13. McDonald, Sharon (ed.), 1993, Inside European Identities. Berg, Providence: Oxford.
14. Peabody, Dean, 1985, National Characteristics, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
15. Schneider, David J., 1996, “Modern Stereotype Research: Unfinished Business” in Macrae, C. Neil, Stangor, Charles and Miles Hewstone (eds.) Stereotypes and Stereotyping, pp. 419-449, New York: The Gilford Press.
16. Segal, Boris, 1987, Russian Drinking. The Use and Abuse of Alcohol in Pre-Revolutionary Russia. New Brunswick: Alcohol Research Documentation. Inc.
17. Ward, Coleen., Bochner, Stephen., Furnham, Adrian, 2001, The Psychology of Culture Shock. London: Routledge.
References
1. Gorshkova V.V., Mel'nikova A.A. Svoeobrazie natsional'noi mental'nosti Rossii kak osnovanie ee tsivilizatsionnoi dinamiki// Sovremennye global'nye vyzovy i natsional'nye interesy. XVI Mezhdunarodnye Likhachevskie nauchnye chteniya. Rossiiskaya akademiya nauk, Kongress peterburgskoi intelligentsii, Sankt-Peterburgskii gumanitarnyi universitet profsoyuzov, pri podderzhke Ministerstva inostrannykh del RF. SPb: iz-vo SPbGUP, 2016. S. 385-386. 656 s.
2. Kas'yanova K. O russkom natsional'nom kharaktere. M.: Akademicheskii Proekt; Ekaterinburg: Delovaya kniga, 2003. – 560 s.
3. Mel'nikova A.A. Yazyk kak tekst natsional'noi kul'tury i forma reprezentatsii ee smyslovykh osnovanii. Dissertatsiya na soiskanie uchenoi stepeni doktora kul'turologii / Sankt-Peterburgskii Gumanitarnyi universitet profsoyuzov. Sankt-Peterburg, 2014. 381 s.
4. Mel'nikova A.A. Yazyk i natsional'nyi kharakter. Vzaimosvyaz' struktury yazyka i mental'nosti. SPb.: Rech', 2003. – 320 s.
5. Pibodi D., Shmelev A.G., Andreeva M.K., Gramenitskii A.E. Psikhosemanticheskii analiz stereotipov russkogo kharaktera: krosskul'turnyi aspekt // Voprosy psikhologii, №3. Moskva, 1993.-S. 101 – 109.
6. Sikevich Z.V. Natsional'noe samosoznanie russkikh. M.: Institut «Otkrytoe obshchestvo», 1996. – 204 s.
7. Smolina T.L. Etnicheskoe samosoznanie kak sotsial'no-psikhologicheskii fenomen // Izvestiya RGPU im. A. I. Gertsena, №171. SPb.: Izdatel'stvo RGPU im. A. I. Gertsena, 2014.-S. 301-306.
8. Smolina T.L., Mel'nikova A.A. Psikhologiya kross-kul'turnoi adaptatsii. SPb.: Izdatel'sko-Torgovyi dom «Skifiya», 2017. – 304 s.
9. Allport, Gordon W., 1979 , The Nature of Prejudice. Reading, Mass.: Addison Wesley.
10. Berdyaev, Nikolay, 1971, Russkaya Ideya (The Russian Idea). Paris: YMCA Press.
11. Gorer G., Rickman J., 1949, The People of Great Russia: a Psychological Study. London.
12. Lippmann, Walter, 1946, Public Opinion. New York: Harcourt Brace.
13. McDonald, Sharon (ed.), 1993, Inside European Identities. Berg, Providence: Oxford.
14. Peabody, Dean, 1985, National Characteristics, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
15. Schneider, David J., 1996, “Modern Stereotype Research: Unfinished Business” in Macrae, C. Neil, Stangor, Charles and Miles Hewstone (eds.) Stereotypes and Stereotyping, pp. 419-449, New York: The Gilford Press.
16. Segal, Boris, 1987, Russian Drinking. The Use and Abuse of Alcohol in Pre-Revolutionary Russia. New Brunswick: Alcohol Research Documentation. Inc.
17. Ward, Coleen., Bochner, Stephen., Furnham, Adrian, 2001, The Psychology of Culture Shock. London: Routledge.
|